Author(s) |
Date |
Details |
Stanley H. Ambrose |
2010 |
Coevolution of Composite-Tool
Technology, Constructive Memory, and Language: Implications for the Evolution
of Modern Human Behavior. In Current Anthropology 51:S1, S135-S147. |
H. Clark Barrett |
2012 |
A hierarchical model of the
evolution of human brain specializations. In Proceedings of the National Academy of
Sciences 109:s1, 10733–10740. |
Roy F.
Baumeister, Liqing Zhang & Kathleen D. Vohs |
2004 |
Gossip as Cultural Learning. In Review of General
Psychology 8:2, 111-121. |
Clay Beckner, Nick C.
Ellis, Richard Blythe, John Holland, Joan Bybee, Jinyun
Ke, Morten H. Christiansen, Diane Larsen-Freeman,
William Croft & Tom Schoenemann |
2009 |
Language Is a Complex Adaptive
System: Position Paper. In Language
Learning 59:S1, 1-26. |
Derek Bickerton |
2014 |
Some Problems for Biolinguistics. In Biolinguistics 8,
73-96. |
Christopher Boehm |
1993 |
Egalitarian Behavior
and Reverse Dominance Hierarchy [and Comments and Reply]. In Current
Anthropology 34:3. 227-254. |
Christophe Boesch &
Hedwige Boesch |
1990 |
Tool Use and Tool Making in Wild
Chimpanzees. In Folia
Primatologica, 54, 86-99. |
Robert Boyd, Peter J. Richerson
& Joseph Henrich |
2011 |
The cultural niche: Why social
learning is essential for human adaptation. In Proceedings of the National Academy of
Sciences, 108:supp.2, 10918-10925. |
Dennis M. Bramble & David R. Carrier |
1983 |
Running and Breathing in Mammals. In Science, 219:4582,
251-256. |
Kyle S. Brown, Curtis W. Marean,
Andy I. R. Herries, Zenobia Jacobs, Chantal Tribolo, David Braun, David L. Roberts, Michael C. Meyer
& Jocelyn Bernatchez |
2009 |
Fire As an Engineering Tool of
Early Modern Humans. In Science
325, 859-862. |
Richard W. Byrne |
2000 |
Evolution of Primate Cognition. In Cognitive Science 24:3,
543-570. |
Richard W. Byrne |
2004 |
The manual skills and cognition
that lie behind hominid tool use. In A.E. Russon &
D.R. Begun (eds.), Evolutionary origins of great ape intelligence.
Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 31-44. |
Michael C. Corballis |
2007 |
The Uniqueness of Human Recursive
Thinking. In American
Scientist 95:3, 240-248. |
T. Dávid-Barrett &
Robin I. M. Dunbar |
2013 |
Processing power limits social
group size: computational evidence for the cognitive costs of sociality. In Proceedings of the Royal
Society B 280, 20131151. |
Barbara L. Davis & Peter F. MacNeilage |
2004 |
The frame/content theory of speech
evolution: from lip smacks to syllables. In Primatologie
6, 305-328. |
Jean Louis Dessalles |
2000 |
Language and Hominid Politics. In C. Knight, J. R. Hurford & M. Studdert-Kennedy
(eds), The Evolutionary emergence of language: social function and the
origins of linguistic form. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, ch4,
62-79. |
Jean Louis Dessalles |
2014 |
Why Talk? In Daniel Dor,
Chris Knight & Jerome Lewis (eds.), The Social Origins of Language.
Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK. |
Robert A. Dielenberg |
2013 |
The comparative psychology of human
uniqueness: A cognitive behavioral review. In Robert G. Bednarik (ed.), The
Psychology of Human Behavior. Nova Science
Publishers: Hauppauge, NY, USA, 111-182. |
Daniel Dor |
2017 |
The role of the lie in the
evolution of human language. In Language Sciences 63, 44-59. |
Robin I. M. Dunbar |
1993 |
Coevolution of neocortical size,
group size and language in humans. In Behavioral
and Brain Sciences 16, 681-735. |
Robin I. M. Dunbar |
2004 |
Mental Magic. In The Human Story: a new history
of mankind's evolution. Faber & Faber Ltd: London, UK, ch3, 41-76. |
Martin P.J.
Edwardes |
2010 |
The Origins of Grammar: An
anthropological perspective. Continuum: London, UK. |
Martin P.J.
Edwardes |
2014 |
Awareness of Self and Awareness of
Selfness: Why the Capacity to Self-Model Represents a Novel Level of
Cognition in Humans. In
G. Rundblad, A. Tytus, O. Knapton & C. Tang
(eds.), Selected Papers from the 4th UK Cognitive Linguistics Conference.
London: UK Cognitive Linguistics Association, 68-83. |
Martin P.J.
Edwardes |
2019 |
The Origins of Self: An
anthropological perspective. UCL Press: London, UK. |
J.M. Engelmann
& M. Tomasello |
2019 |
Children’s Sense of Fairness as
Equal Respect. In Trends
in Cognitive Sciences 23:6, 454-463. |
David Erdal &
Andrew Whiten |
1994 |
On Human Egalitarianism: An
Evolutionary Product of Machiavellian Status Escalation? In Current Anthropology,
35:2, 175-183. |
James H. Fowler & Nicholas A. Christakis |
2010 |
Cooperative behavior
cascades in human social networks. In Proceedings of the National Academy of
Sciences 107:12, 5334-5338. |
Patricia A.
Ganea |
2005 |
Contextual Factors Affect Absent
Reference Comprehension in 14-Month-Olds. In Child Development 76:5, 989-998. |
Thomas Geissmann |
2000 |
Gibbon songs and human music from
an evolutionary perspective. In M.L. Wallin, B. Merker & S. Brown (eds.), The Origins of Music. MIT
Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 103-123. |
John A.J. Gowlett & Richard W. Wrangham |
2013 |
Earliest fire in Africa: towards
the convergence of archaeological evidence and the cooking hypothesis. In Azania: Archaeological
Research in Africa 48:1, 5-30. |
Philipp Gunz, Simon
Neubauer, Dean Falk, Paul Tafforeau, Adeline Le Cabec, Tanya M. Smith, William H. Kimbel,
Fred Spoor & Zeresenay Alemseged |
2020 |
Australopithecus afarensis
endocasts suggest ape-like brain organization and prolonged brain growth. In Science Advances 2020:6,
eaaz4729. |
Brian Hare & Michael Tomasello |
2004 |
Chimpanzees are more skilful in
competitive than in cooperative cognitive tasks. In Animal Behaviour 68,
571-581. |
Marc D. Hauser, Noam Chomsky & W. Tecumseh
Fitch |
2002 |
The Faculty of Language: What Is
It, Who Has It, and How Did It Evolve? In Science, 298, 1569-1579. |
Bernd Heine
& Kyung-An Song |
2010 |
On the genesis of personal
pronouns: Some conceptual sources. In Language and Cognition 2:1, 117-147. |
J. Henrich
& F.J. Gil-White |
2001 |
The evolution of prestige: freely conferred
deference as a mechanism for enhancing the benefits of cultural transmission. In Evolution and Human Behavior 22:3, 165-196. |
James R. Hurford |
2004 |
Human uniqueness, learned symbols and recursive
thought. In European
Review 12:4, 551-565. |
James R. Hurford |
2008 |
The evolution of human
communication and language. In Patrizia D'Ettorre & David Hughes (eds.), Sociobiology
of Communication: an interdisciplinary perspective. Oxford University
Press: Oxford, UK, 249-264. |
Chris Knight & Jerome Lewis |
2014 |
Vocal Deception, Laughter, and the
Linguistic Significance of Reverse Dominance. In Daniel Dor,
Chris Knight & Jerome Lewis (eds.), The Social Origins of Language.
Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK. |
C. Knight,
C. Power & I. Watts |
1995 |
The Human Symbolic Revolution: A
Darwinian Account. In Cambridge
Archaeological Journal 5:1 (1995), pp. 75-114 |
Steven L. Kuhn |
1992 |
On Planning and Curated
Technologies in the Middle Paleolithic. In Journal of Anthropological
Research 48:3, 185-214. |
Margaret S. Livingstone, Warren W. Pettine,
Krishna Srihasam, Brandon Moore, Istvan A. Morocz & Daeyeol Lee |
2014 |
Symbol addition by monkeys provides
evidence for normalized quantity coding. In PNAS, 111:18, 6822-6827. |
Salikoko Mufwene |
2013 |
Language as Technology: Some
questions that evolutionary linguistics should address. In T. Lohndal
(ed.), In Search of Universal Grammar: From Norse to Zoque.
John Benjamins: Amsterdam, Netherlands. |
Robert R. Provine |
2004 |
Laughing, Tickling, and the
Evolution of Speech and Self. In Current Directions in Psychological
Science 13:6, 215-218. |
Jill D. Pruetz & Thomas C. LaDuke |
2010 |
Reaction to Fire by Savanna Chimpanzees
(Pan troglodytes verus) at Fongoli,
Senegal. In American
Journal of Physical Anthropology 141:4, 646-50. |
Anne Reboul |
2021 |
Truthfully Misleading: Truth,
Informativity, and Manipulation in Linguistic Communication. In Frontiers in Communication
6:646820. |
Daniel L.
Schacter, Donna Rose Addis and Randy L. Buckner |
2007 |
Remembering the past to imagine the
future: the prospective brain. In Nature Reviews Neuroscience 8, 657-661. |
Jay Schulkin &
Greta B. Raglan |
2014 |
The evolution of music and human
social capability. In Frontiers
in Neuroscience 8:292 |
Thom Scott-Phillips |
2010 |
The Evolution of Communication:
Humans may be exceptional. In Interactive
Studies 11:1, 78-99. |
Sileshi Semaw |
2000 |
The World’s Oldest Stone Artefacts
from Gona, Ethiopia: Their Implications for
Understanding Stone Technology and Patterns of Human Evolution Between
2·6–1·5 Million Years Ago. In Journal
of Archaeological Science 27, 1197-1214. |
Robert M. Seyfarth, Dorothy L. Cheney & Peter
Marler |
1980 |
Vervet Monkey Alarm Calls: Semantic
Communication in a Free-ranging Primate. In Animal Behaviour 28, 1070-1094. |
Robert M. Seyfarth, Joan B. Silk & Dorothy L.
Cheney |
2014 |
Social bonds in female baboons: the
interaction between personality, kinship and rank. In Animal Behaviour 87,
23-29. |
Ainara Sistiaga,
Fatima Husain, David Uribelarrea, David M. Martín-Perea, Troy Ferland, Katherine
H. Freeman, Fernando Diez-Martín, Enrique Baquedano,
Audax Mabulla, Manuel Domínguez-Rodrigo & Roger
E. Summons |
2020 |
Microbial biomarkers reveal a
hydrothermally active landscape at Olduvai Gorge at the dawn of the
Acheulean, 1.7 Ma. In Proceedings
of the National Academy of Sciences 117:40, 24720-24728. |
Kim Sterelny |
2016 |
Cumulative Cultural Evolution and
the Origins of Language. In Biological
Theory 11, 173-186. |
Dietrich Stout & Thierry Chaminade |
2009 |
Making Tools and Making Sense:
Complex, Intentional Behaviour in Human Evolution. In Cambridge Archaeological
Journal 19:1, 85-96. |
Michael Tomasello,
Malinda Carpenter, Josep Call, Tanya Behne & Henrike Moll |
2005 |
Understanding and sharing
intentions: The origins of cultural cognition. In Behavioral
and Brain Sciences (2005) 28, 675-735. |
Polly W. Wiessner |
2014 |
Embers of society: Firelight talk among
the Ju/’hoansi Bushmen. In Proceedings of the National
Academy of Sciences 111:39, 14027-14035. |
Richard Wrangham |
2016 |
The curiously long absence of
cooking in evolutionary thought. In Learning Behavior
44, 116-117. |