Author(s) |
Date |
Details |
Stanley H. Ambrose |
2010 |
Coevolution of Composite-Tool
Technology, Constructive Memory, and Language: Implications for the Evolution
of Modern Human Behavior. Current Anthropology 51:S1, S135-S147. |
H. Clark Barrett |
2012 |
A
hierarchical model of the evolution of human brain specializations. Proceedings
of the National Academy of Sciences 109:s1,
10733–10740. |
Roy F.
Baumeister, Liqing Zhang & Kathleen D. Vohs |
2004 |
Gossip as Cultural Learning. Review of General Psychology
8:2, 111-121. |
Clay Beckner, Nick C. Ellis, Richard Blythe, John
Holland, Joan Bybee, Jinyun Ke, Morten H.
Christiansen, Diane Larsen-Freeman, William Croft & Tom Schoenemann |
2009 |
Language Is a Complex Adaptive
System: Position Paper. Language
Learning 59:S1, 1-26. |
Michel Belyk, Rachel
Brown, Deryk S. Beal, Alard Roebroeck, Carolyn
McGettigan, Stella Guldner & Sonja A. Kotz |
2021 |
Human larynx motor cortices
coordinate respiration for vocal-motor control. Neuroimage 239, 118326 |
Antonio Benítez-Burraco |
2017 |
Figurative Language, Language
Disorders, and Language(s) Evolution. Frontiers in Psychology, 8:1713. |
Silje Evjenth Bentsen |
2014 |
Using Pyrotechnology:
Fire-related Features and Activities with a Focus on the African Middle Stone
Age. Journal
of Archaeological Research 22, 141-175. |
Derek Bickerton |
2014 |
Some Problems for Biolinguistics. Biolinguistics 8, 73-96. |
Christopher Boehm |
1993 |
Egalitarian Behavior
and Reverse Dominance Hierarchy [and Comments and Reply]. Current
Anthropology 34:3. 227-254. |
Christophe Boesch & Hedwige Boesch |
1990 |
Tool Use and Tool Making in Wild
Chimpanzees. Folia
Primatologica, 54, 86-99. |
Robert Boyd, Peter J. Richerson & Joseph
Henrich |
2011 |
The cultural niche: Why social
learning is essential for human adaptation. Proceedings of the National Academy of
Sciences, 108:supp.2, 10918-10925. |
Dennis M. Bramble & David R. Carrier |
1983 |
Running and Breathing in Mammals. Science, 219:4582,
251-256. |
Kyle S. Brown, Curtis W. Marean, Andy I. R. Herries, Zenobia Jacobs, Chantal Tribolo,
David Braun, David L. Roberts, Michael C. Meyer & Jocelyn Bernatchez |
2009 |
Fire As an Engineering Tool of
Early Modern Humans. Science
325, 859-862. |
Richard W. Byrne |
2000 |
Evolution of Primate Cognition. Cognitive Science 24:3,
543-570. |
Richard W. Byrne |
2004 |
The manual skills and cognition
that lie behind hominid tool use. In A.E. Russon & D.R. Begun (eds.), Evolutionary
origins of great ape intelligence. Cambridge University Press: Cambridge,
UK, 31-44. |
Susana
Carvalho & Megan Beardmore-Herd |
2019 |
Technological Origins: Primate
Perspectives and Early Hominin Tool Use in Africa. In The Oxford Research Encyclopedia, African History. https://oxfordre.com/africanhistory/display/10.1093/acrefore/9780190277734.001.0001/acrefore-9780190277734-e-75
|
Michael C. Corballis |
2007 |
The Uniqueness of Human Recursive
Thinking. American
Scientist 95:3, 240-248. |
T. Dávid-Barrett & Robin I. M. Dunbar |
2013 |
Processing power limits social
group size: computational evidence for the cognitive costs of sociality. Proceedings of the Royal
Society B 280, 20131151. |
Barbara L. Davis & Peter F. MacNeilage |
2004 |
The frame/content theory of speech
evolution: from lip smacks to syllables. Primatologie
6, 305-328. |
Jean Louis Dessalles |
2000 |
Language and Hominid Politics. In C. Knight, M. Studdert-Kennedy
& J. R. Hurford (eds.), The Evolutionary Emergence of Language: social
function and the origins of linguistic form. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge
University Press. |
Jean Louis Dessalles |
2014 |
The Role of the Human Political
Singularity in the Emergence of Language. In E.A. Cartmill, S. Roberts, H. Lyn & H.
Cornish (eds.), The Evolution of Language: Proceedings of the 10th
International Conference (Evolang-X - Vienna),
World Scientific, 423-424. |
Robert A. Dielenberg |
2013 |
The comparative psychology of human
uniqueness: A cognitive behavioral review. In Robert G. Bednarik (ed.), The
Psychology of Human Behavior. Nova Science
Publishers: Hauppauge, NY, USA, 111-182. |
Daniel Dor |
2017 |
The role of the lie in the
evolution of human language. Language Sciences 63, 44-59. |
Robin I. M. Dunbar |
1993 |
Coevolution of neocortical size,
group size and language in humans. Behavioral
and Brain Sciences 16, 681-735. |
Robin I. M. Dunbar |
2004 |
Mental Magic. In The Human Story: a new
history of mankind's evolution. Faber & Faber Ltd: London, UK, ch3,
41-76. |
Martin P.J.
Edwardes |
2014 |
Awareness of Self and Awareness of Selfness: Why the Capacity to Self-Model Represents a
Novel Level of Cognition in Humans. In G. Rundblad, A. Tytus, O. Knapton & C.
Tang (eds.), Selected Papers from the 4th UK Cognitive Linguistics
Conference. London: UK Cognitive Linguistics Association, 68-83. |
Martin P.J.
Edwardes |
2019 |
Where did self
come from? In
The Origins of Self: An anthropological perspective, UCL Press:
London, UK, ch2, 29-51. |
Martin P.J.
Edwardes |
2024 |
Modelling Society and Sharing
Models. In The
Sources of Language Grammar: An anthropological perspective. Scitsiugnil Press: London, UK, ch7, 157-177. |
J.M. Engelmann
& M. Tomasello |
2019 |
Children’s Sense of Fairness as
Equal Respect. Trends
in Cognitive Sciences 23:6, 454-463. |
David Erdal & Andrew Whiten |
1994 |
On Human Egalitarianism: An
Evolutionary Product of Machiavellian Status Escalation? Current Anthropology, 35:2,
175-183. |
James H. Fowler & Nicholas A. Christakis |
2010 |
Cooperative behavior
cascades in human social networks. Proceedings of the National Academy of
Sciences 107:12, 5334-5338. |
Chris D.
Frith |
2012 |
The role of metacognition in human
social interactions. Philosophical
Transactions of the Royal Society B 367, 2213-2223. |
Patricia A.
Ganea |
2005 |
Contextual Factors Affect Absent
Reference Comprehension in 14-Month-Olds. Child Development 76:5, 989-998. |
Thomas Geissmann |
2000 |
Gibbon songs and human music from
an evolutionary perspective. In M.L. Wallin, B. Merker & S. Brown
(eds.), The Origins of Music. MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 103-123. |
John A.J. Gowlett & Richard W. Wrangham |
2013 |
Earliest fire in Africa: towards
the convergence of archaeological evidence and the cooking hypothesis. Azania: Archaeological
Research in Africa 48:1, 5-30. |
Philipp Gunz, Simon Neubauer, Dean Falk, Paul Tafforeau, Adeline Le Cabec,
Tanya M. Smith, William H. Kimbel, Fred Spoor & Zeresenay Alemseged |
2020 |
Australopithecus afarensis
endocasts suggest ape-like brain organization and prolonged brain growth. Science Advances 2020:6,
eaaz4729. |
William E.H. Harcourt-Smith |
2013 |
The Origins of Bipedal Locomotion. In Winfried Henke & Ian
Tattersall (eds.), Handbook of Paleoanthropology.
Springer-Verlag: Berlin, Germany, Phylogeny of Hominids ch5, 1483-1518. |
Brian Hare & Michael Tomasello |
2004 |
Chimpanzees are more skilful in
competitive than in cooperative cognitive tasks. Animal Behaviour 68,
571-581. |
Marc D. Hauser, Noam Chomsky & W. Tecumseh
Fitch |
2002 |
The Faculty of Language: What Is
It, Who Has It, and How Did It Evolve? Science, 298, 1569-1579. |
Kristen Hawkes
& James E. Coxworth |
2013 |
Grandmothers and the Evolution of
Human Longevity: A Review of Findings and Future Directions. Evolutionary Anthropology
22, 294-302. |
Bernd Heine
& Kyung-An Song |
2010 |
On the genesis of personal
pronouns: Some conceptual sources. Language and Cognition 2:1, 117-147. |
J. Henrich
& F.J. Gil-White |
2001 |
The evolution of prestige: freely
conferred deference as a mechanism for enhancing the benefits of cultural
transmission. Evolution
and Human Behavior 22:3, 165-196. |
Erella Hovers |
2012 |
Invention, reinvention and
innovation: the makings of Oldowan lithic
technology. In
Scott Elias (ed.), Origins of Human Innovation and Creativity.
Elsevier: Amsterdam, Netherlands. (Developments in Quaternary Science 16,
ch5, 51-68). |
James R. Hurford |
2004 |
Human uniqueness, learned symbols and recursive
thought. European
Review 12:4, 551-565. |
James R. Hurford |
2008 |
The evolution of human
communication and language. In Patrizia D'Ettorre & David Hughes
(eds.), Sociobiology of Communication: an interdisciplinary perspective.
Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 249-264. |
Marthe Kiley-Worthington |
2017 |
The Mental Homologies of Mammals.
Towards an Understanding of Another Mammal’s World View. Animals 7:87. |
Chris Knight & Jerome Lewis |
2014 |
Vocal Deception, Laughter, and the
Linguistic Significance of Reverse Dominance. In Daniel Dor, Chris Knight &
Jerome Lewis (eds.), The Social Origins of Language. Oxford University
Press: Oxford, UK. |
C. Knight,
C. Power & I. Watts |
1995 |
The Human Symbolic Revolution: A
Darwinian Account. Cambridge
Archaeological Journal 5:1 (1995), pp. 75-114 |
Steven L. Kuhn |
1992 |
On Planning and Curated
Technologies in the Middle Paleolithic. Journal of Anthropological
Research 48:3, 185-214. |
George Lakoff |
1993 |
Conceptual Metaphor: the
contemporary theory of metaphor. In Andrew Ortony
(ed.), Metaphor and Thought, Cambridge University Press, 202-251. |
Margaret S. Livingstone, Warren W. Pettine,
Krishna Srihasam, Brandon Moore, Istvan A. Morocz & Daeyeol Lee |
2014 |
Symbol addition by monkeys provides
evidence for normalized quantity coding. Proceedings of the National Academy of
Sciences 111:18, 6822-6827. |
Maria A. Matos & Maria de Lourdes Passos |
2010 |
Emergent Verbal Behavior
and Analogy: Skinnerian and Linguistic Approaches. The Behavior
Analyst 33,
65-81. |
Ryan McKay & Harvey Whitehouse |
2015 |
Religion and Morality. Psychological Bulletin
141:2, 447-473. |
Salikoko S. Mufwene |
2013 |
Language as Technology: Some
questions that evolutionary linguistics should address. In T. Lohndal
(ed.), In Search of Universal Grammar: From Norse to Zoque.
John Benjamins: Amsterdam, Netherlands. |
Salikoko S. Mufwene |
2023 |
Linguistic Hybridization in the
Emergence of Creoles. The
Cambridge Journal of Postcolonial Literary Inquiry, 10:1, 74-89. |
Lauri Nummenmaa, Enrico
Glerean, Mikko Viinikainen, Iiro P. Jääskeläinen,
Riitta Haria, & Mikko Sams |
2012 |
Emotions promote social interaction
by synchronizing brain activity across individuals. Proceedings of the National Academy
of Sciences
109:24, 9599-9604. |
Robert R. Provine |
2004 |
Laughing, Tickling, and the Evolution
of Speech and Self. Current
Directions in Psychological Science 13:6, 215-218. |
Jill D. Pruetz & Thomas C. LaDuke |
2010 |
Reaction to Fire by Savanna
Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes verus) at Fongoli, Senegal. American Journal of Physical Anthropology
141:4, 646-50. |
Anne Reboul |
2021 |
Truthfully Misleading: Truth,
Informativity, and Manipulation in Linguistic Communication. Frontiers in Communication
6:646820. |
Sandra
Reinert, Mark Hübener, Tobias Bonhoeffer &
Pieter M. Goltstein |
2021 |
Mouse prefrontal cortex represents
learned rules for categorization. Nature 593, 411-417. |
Daniel L.
Schacter, Donna Rose Addis & Randy L. Buckner |
2007 |
Remembering the past to imagine the
future: the prospective brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 8, 657-661. |
Jay Schulkin & Greta B. Raglan |
2014 |
The evolution of music and human
social capability. Frontiers
in Neuroscience 8:292 |
Thom Scott-Phillips |
2010 |
The Evolution of Communication:
Humans may be exceptional. Interactive
Studies 11:1, 78-99. |
Sileshi Semaw |
2000 |
The World’s Oldest Stone Artefacts
from Gona, Ethiopia: Their Implications for Understanding Stone Technology
and Patterns of Human Evolution Between 2·6–1·5 Million
Years Ago. Journal
of Archaeological Science 27, 1197-1214. |
Robert M. Seyfarth, Dorothy L. Cheney & Peter
Marler |
1980 |
Vervet Monkey Alarm Calls: Semantic
Communication in a Free-ranging Primate. Animal Behaviour 28, 1070-1094. |
Robert M. Seyfarth, Joan B. Silk & Dorothy L.
Cheney |
2014 |
Social bonds in female baboons: the
interaction between personality, kinship and rank. Animal Behaviour 87,
23-29. |
C. Shipton |
2018 |
Biface Knapping Skill in the East
African Acheulean: Progressive Trends and Random Walks. African Archaeological Review 35, 107-131. |
Ainara Sistiaga, Fatima
Husain, David Uribelarrea, David M. Martín-Perea,
Troy Ferland, Katherine H. Freeman, Fernando Diez-Martín, Enrique Baquedano,
Audax Mabulla, Manuel Domínguez-Rodrigo & Roger
E. Summons |
2020 |
Microbial biomarkers reveal a
hydrothermally active landscape at Olduvai Gorge at the dawn of the
Acheulean, 1.7 Ma. Proceedings
of the National Academy of Sciences 117:40, 24720-24728. |
Andrew C.
Sorensen |
2019 |
The Uncertain Origins of Fire-Making
by Humans: The State of the Art and Smouldering Questions. Mitteilungen der Gesellschaft für Urgeschichte 28, 11-50. |
Kim Sterelny |
2016 |
Cumulative Cultural Evolution and
the Origins of Language. Biological
Theory 11, 173-186. |
Dietrich Stout & Thierry Chaminade |
2009 |
Making Tools and Making Sense:
Complex, Intentional Behaviour in Human Evolution. Cambridge Archaeological
Journal 19:1, 85-96. |
Michael Tomasello, Malinda Carpenter, Josep Call,
Tanya Behne & Henrike Moll |
2005 |
Understanding and sharing
intentions: The origins of cultural cognition. Behavioral
and Brain Sciences (2005) 28, 675-735. |
Mark White and Nick Ashton |
2003 |
Lower Palaeolithic Core Technology
and the Origins of the Levallois Method in North-Western Europe. Current Anthropology 44:4,
598-609. |
Polly W. Wiessner |
2014 |
Embers of society: Firelight talk among
the Ju/’hoansi Bushmen. Proceedings of the National
Academy of Sciences 111:39, 14027-14035. |
Richard Wrangham |
2016 |
The curiously long absence of
cooking in evolutionary thought. Learning Behavior
44, 116-117. |